OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine is an international peer-reviewed Open Access journal published quarterly online by LIDSEN Publishing Inc. It covers all evidence-based scientific studies on integrative, alternative and complementary approaches to improving health and wellness.

Topics contain but are not limited to:

  • Acupuncture
  • Acupressure
  • Acupotomy
  • Bioelectromagnetics applications
  • Pharmacological and biological treatments including their efficacy and safety
  • Diet, nutrition and lifestyle changes
  • Herbal medicine
  • Homeopathy
  • Manual healing methods (e.g., massage, physical therapy)
  • Kinesiology
  • Mind/body interventions
  • Preventive medicine
  • Research in integrative medicine
  • Education in integrative medicine
  • Related policies

The journal publishes a variety of article types: Original Research, Review, Communication, Opinion, Comment, Conference Report, Technical Note, Book Review, etc.

There is no restriction on paper length, provided that the text is concise and comprehensive. Authors should present their results in as much detail as possible, as reviewers are encouraged to emphasize scientific rigor and reproducibility.

Publication Speed (median values for papers published in 2023): Submission to First Decision: 5.9 weeks; Submission to Acceptance: 14.7 weeks; Acceptance to Publication: 8 days (1-2 days of FREE language polishing included)

Open Access Research Article

Electrodermal Correlates of Hypnosis: Current Developments

Krisztian Kasos 1, 2, Luca Csirmaz 2, Fanni Vikor 2, Szabolcs Zimonyi 2, Katalin Varga 2, Anna Szekely 2

  1. Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

  2. MTA-ELTE Lendület Adaptation Research Group, Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

Correspondence: Krisztian Kasos

Academic Editor: Giuseppe De Benedittis

Special Issue: Hypnosis: from Neural Mechanisms to Clinical Practice

Received: February 07, 2020 | Accepted: March 23, 2020 | Published: April 01, 2020

OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine 2020, Volume 5, Issue 2, doi:10.21926/obm.icm.2002017

Recommended citation: Kasos K, Csirmaz L, Vikor F, Zimonyi S, Varga K, Szekely A. Electrodermal Correlates of Hypnosis: Current Developments. OBM Integrative and Complementary Medicine 2020; 5(2): 017; doi:10.21926/obm.icm.2002017.

© 2020 by the authors. This is an open access article distributed under the conditions of the Creative Commons by Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, provided the original work is correctly cited.

Abstract

Hypnosis has proven to be an effective treatment in disorders that affect the autonomic nervous system (ANS). However, the studies investigating the nature of its effect on the ANS have reported contradictory results. Measurement of electrodermal activity (EDA) is an objective way to assess the activity of the sympathetic branch of the ANS. We aim to elucidate the effects of hypnosis on EDA. Here, we report the results of two studies, both investigating the psychophysiological effects of hypnosis.In the first experiment, subjects engaged in an HGSHS:A group hypnosis session to measure their hypnotizability. EDA was measured bilaterally from their wrists. We found a significant reduction in EDA levels and the number of nonspecific responses during the hypnotic induction phase. This effect was observed in all three hypnotizability groups—high, medium, and low hypnotizables.
A three-way interaction confirmed that EDA patterns on the left and right sides were characteristically different in these three groups. Left-side dominance was typical in high hypnotizables, whereas low hypnotizables were characteristically right-sided. EDA levels of the two sides remained synchronous in medium hypnotizables. During the suggestion phase, we found significant differences in EDA levels depending on the test suggestions, modulated by hypnotizability. A suggestion, harder to respond to, elicited higher arousal in high hypnotizables as compared to low hypnotizables.

In the second experiment, we performed five consecutive hypnosis sessions to confirm the reproducibility of the most prominent effect found in Study 1—a gradual decrease in the level of skin conductance during hypnotic induction. We also confirmed that this effect is independent of the hypnotizability level.

We conclude that arousal is bilaterally reduced during hypnosis induction, which is persistent across different levels of hypnotizability. At the same time, lateral differences define unique EDA patterns in the induction phase, characterizing high, medium, and low hypnotizables.

Keywords

Bilateral; electrodermal; EDA; group measurement; hypnosis 

1. Introduction

Hypnosis is a state of consciousness that is characterized by focused attention and decreased peripheral awareness, accompanied by an increased capacity to respond to suggestions [1]. During hypnosis, the subjects often report changes in time sense, body image, memory, self-awareness, and volitional control, all associated with an altered state of consciousness [2]. Hypnosis is a product of the procedure called hypnotic induction [1]. A hypnotic state is achieved when individuals respond to suggestions in an automatic fashion, ignoring environmental stimuli, other than those pointed out by the hypnotist. In this state, the individual tends to see, feel, and smell in accordance with the hypnotist’s suggestions, even though these suggestions may be in contradiction to the actual stimuli present in the environment. The degree to which people respond to suggestions is called hypnotizability. It is typically measured on a scale ranging from low to high. It is a stable, trait-like characteristic that does not change significantly over time; measured 25 years later, the correlation remains high with r = 0.75 [3]. We usually divide people into three groups—low, medium, and high hypnotizable individuals. Low and high hypnotizables are often compared in research as the two ends of the continuum [4]. There are standardized procedures to induce hypnosis and to measure hypnotizability. They involve rapport building followed by a hypnotic induction, various test suggestions, and a deinduction [5,6].

Hypnosis has long been known as a useful therapeutic tool for various psychological and physiological disorders, including chronic headaches, hypertension, and various forms of anxiety [7]. It is particularly efficient for disorders that are characterized by changes in the autonomic nervous system. The reason for this efficacy may lie in the reduction in psychophysiological arousal and the modulation of autonomic activity [8,9,10]. Forbes and Pekala (1993)[8] reported that self-hypnosis training produced psychological improvements associated with reduced anxiety, reduced pulse rate, and increased skin temperature [8]. Kanji and colleagues (2006)[11] demonstrated that eight sessions of autogenic training lowered state and trait anxiety levels as well as systolic and diastolic blood pressure [11]. Hypnosis can also be an adjuvant treatment for major depression, a disorder that is associated with autonomic nervous system changes, such as decreased heart rate variability [12]. Chen and colleagues (2017) [12] found that heart rate parameters significantly improved in the hypnotic and post-hypnotic conditions compared to the pre-hypnotic condition. Thus, they concluded that hypnotic treatment might bring improvements in vegetative functions [12].

Measuring electrodermal activity can be an unobtrusive and cost-effective way to gain information about the autonomic nervous system [13]. The ease of use and the widely available technology have made the measurement of electrodermal activity (EDA) a popular tool in hypnosis research. Tools measuring skin conductance (SC) make use of the eccrine sweat glands, which are exclusively innervated by the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) [14,15]. Thus, the tonic component of skin conductance, skin conductance level (SCL), is an excellent way to gauge the background level of the SNS. In contrast, the phasic component, skin conductance response (SCR), provides information about the autonomic responses to the given stimuli. The tonic component is characterized by slow changes; whereas, the phasic components change faster. Dawson and colleagues provided a detailed description of the different components of skin conductance [15,16]. Determining the measurement sites for EDA is important since the density of eccrine sweat glands differs in different parts of the body. The most responsive sites to measure electrodermal activity are the palmar and plantar surfaces (for a recent bilateral analysis of traditional and alternate measuring sites, see Kasos et al., under review).

In the following section, we summarized the research results concerning the relationship between EDA and hypnosis. The studies are listed in chronological order.

Table 1 Summary of research results regarding EDA and hypnosis.

The studies are listed in chronological order. “N” –number of participants; “Uni” –unilateral measurement; “Bi”–bilateral measurements; HGSHS – Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility [17]; WSGC–Waterloo Stanford Group Scale [5]; SHSS–Stanford hypnotic Susceptibility Scale [6].

The characteristic points related to hypnosis and EDA, based on the above results, may be summarized as below:

  1. Regarding EDA levels, eight studies reported lower skin conductance during hypnosis compared to pre-hypnosis, post-hypnosis, or control conditions. Only one study observed a higher level of skin conductance during the hypnotic induction. Three studies found no difference between the skin conductance levels in hypnosis and control condition.

  2. Many studies reported that the number of skin conductance responses (SCRs) or nonspecific SCRs were fewer during hypnosis than in control conditions. Others found that high hypnotizable individuals had less nonspecific SCRs than the low hypnotizable subjects. SCRs have smaller amplitudes in hypnosis, which is more prominent in high hypnotizables.

  3. A research group published two studies with contradictory results regarding bilateral EDA.

1.1 Methodology in Hypnosis Research

Hypnosis research is riddled with methodological diversity. It would be most effective to use a standard induction procedure to ensure the reproducibility of results. Using standardized scales for measuring hypnotizability would also be beneficial to compare results.

From Table 1, it is clear that the standardized methodology of electrodermal measurements and reporting would be beneficial. Dawson (2007) [16] recommended taking the measurements from the distal phalanges of the index and middle fingers of the non-dominant hand. If those are unavailable, current studies have reported alternative measurement sites (see Kasos et al., under review). In the present study, we took the measurements from the wrists, as some of the test suggestions required use of both the hands, including fingers. SCR window should be set between 1 and 5 s after stimulus onset [15]. The minimum threshold for SCR amplitude should be set to the recommended 0.01 µS [14,15].

Based on the above methodology, we hypothesized that:

  1. SCL will reduce during the full hypnotic induction phase.

  2. There will be fewer SCRs at the end of the induction compared to the beginning of the induction.

  3. The above differences will be more prominent in those who are more susceptible.

  4. There will be lateral differences in EDA during the hypnotic induction and during test suggestions, modulated by hypnotizability.

We performed a follow-up study to demonstrate that the most prominent effects found in Study 1 are reproducible.

2. Methods

2.1 Study 1

We recruited 38 university students as our subjects (N = 38, Mean age = 21.11, SD = 1.75), who were right-handed and had no prior experience in hypnosis. Exclusion criteria included the presence of mental illness and the use of drugs and alcohol, based on self-reporting. All participants were Hungarians (Caucasian).

Procedure: Participants were invited to take part in a group hypnosis session, where their hypnotizability was measured with the HGSHS: A (Költő, 2015). On arrival, the participants were asked to fill out an informed consent form and briefed about the experiment. We attached the electrodermal sensors to their left and right wrists. The wrists were chosen as an alternative to the traditional palmar locations because certain test suggestions required both to be close together (finger lock and hands moving together), which could cause unwanted artifacts. Participants were asked to sit comfortably but as still as possible, to avoid movement throughout the EDA measurement. A certified hypnotist read the hypnosis script, in the presence of a co-hypnotist. After the hypnosis session, EDA sensors were removed, and the participants were asked to fill out our questionnaires. At the end, the participants were debriefed.

2.2 Study 2

We recruited 19 Hungarian university students as subjects (N = 19, Mean age = 21.58, SD = 4.07), who received course credit for their participation. This optional course was about test-anxiety reduction techniques, and one of the techniques, they could experience was hypnosis. Exclusion criteria included the presence of mental illness and the use of drugs and alcohol, based on self-reporting.

Procedure: participants filled out an informed consent form, in the beginning of the semester. Their hypnotizability was measured with the HGSHS:A on the same day. On the following five occasions, each two-weeks apart, electrodermal sensors were attached to the proximal phalanges of the middle and index fingers of their non-dominant hand. They were asked to sit as still as possible to avoid movement during the measurements. An audio-recorded hypnosis script was played for the participants. In all five sessions, hypnosis was induced according to the Hungarian version of the Stanford Clinical Scale (SCS) (Morgan and Hilgard, 1978–1979), followed by suggestions with the purpose of reducing test-anxiety. The SCS induction was chosen because it is shorter than the HGSHS used in the first experiment. It was an important criterion in keeping the interventions short. The hypnosis sessions lasted between 17 and 20 min. Once the recording was over, electrodermal sensors were removed.

2.3 Data Collection and Processing

Hypnotizability scores were based on participants’ reactions to the suggestions in the hypnosis session. Based on the scores, they were divided into three hypnotizability groups—i) low hypnotizables with scores 4 or below, ii) medium hypnotizables with scores between 5 and 8, and iii) high hypnotizables with scores of 9 and above.

First, we measured raw skin conductivity every 125 ms for the first 10 min of the hypnosis session (induction phase) and during suggestions (hypnosis phase). EDA was analyzed in Ledalab 3.4.8 [18]. For smoothing, a Gaussian window was applied. SCL was extracted by optimized continuous decomposition analysis.

Next, we calculated subject-independent EDA measures for the detailed analyses of the induction phase of study 1. We aimed to reduce individual variability in electrodermal levels to detect lateral changes with time, a characteristic of the three hypnotizability groups. Thus, data were standardized within individuals, using the average SCL values and the standard deviations (SD) of the 2 × 480 data points of induction phase from both wrists, to calculate the Z-scored EDA for every raw data point. Similarly, the number of SCRs was also standardized (Z-scored SCR) within individuals, using the number of SCRs in every two minutes of the induction phase measured from both wrists. The average number and SDs of SCR counts within the 2-minute intervals were used for calculating Z-scores.

Then, we calculated the laterality coefficient. This procedure standardized the values between −1 and +1. Negative numbers represent right side dominance, and positive numbers represent left side dominance [4].

Finally, we applied the analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test the effects of time, suggestions, hypnotizability, and laterality on psychophysiological responses during the induction/suggestion phase. The following EDA measures were dependent variables—Z-scored SCL, laterality coefficient values based on average SCL in every 2 minutes, and z-scored number of SCRs for each 2 minutes. We tested the within subject factors, time and side (left and right), as well as the between subject factor, hypnotizability (low, medium, and high).

3. Result

3.1 Study 1

3.1.1 Detailed Analyses of the Skin Conductance Level of the Hypnosis Induction

In the hypnosis induction phase, a standard set of preliminary instructions and suggestions are communicated to the individuals being hypnotized. The way people reach or fail to reach the hypnotic state is of vital importance; thus, we decided to analyze EDA responses to the first 10 min of the induction phase in a detailed fashion.

Based on the literature, we hypothesized a reduction in SCL during the hypnotic induction, especially in those who score high on the hypnotizability scale. The three-way mixed ANOVA on SCL during the 10-minute induction phase using side (left/right) and time as within-subject factors and hypnotizability (low/medium/high) as a between-subject factor, resulted in a prominent effect of time with F (4,140) = 2.65, p = 0.036, ηp2 = 0.07. The level of skin conductance decreased on both sides during the induction process in all three groups. Figure 1 depicts changes in Z-scored SCL during induction, averaged for the left and right hands of the three hypnotizability groups. There were no other main effects.

There were no significant two-way interactions. The analysis resulted in a significant three-way interaction of side, time, and hypnotizability with F (8,140) = 2.49, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.13. Low, medium, and high hypnotizables showed characteristically different EDA patterns on their left and right sides (Figure 1). The low hypnotizable individuals displayed right-side dominance, while high hypnotizable individuals displayed left-side dominance throughout the induction phase. On the contrary, the left- and right-side SCLs were similar in medium hypnotizables. High and medium hypnotizables showed lower EDA variability compared to that in the low hypnotizables. For the medium hypnotizables, SCL gradually decreased throughout the 10 minutes of induction phase. On the other hand, both low and high hypnotizable individuals showed variable EDA patterns within this timeframe.

Click to view original image

Figure 1 EDA during the induction (10 minutes). Black lines represent EDA measured from the right wrist. Grey lines represent EDA measured from the left wrist.

3.1.2 NonSpecific Responses

We predicted that fewer SCRs would characterize the end of the induction phase compared to the beginning of the induction. Findings from the literature also suggest less nonspecific SCRs in EDA patterns of high hypnotizables as compared to the low hypnotizables. Three-way mixed ANOVA analysis was performed on the number of Z-scored SCRs for every two minutes of the induction phase. We used time and side as within-subject factors, and hypnotizability (low/medium/high) as a between-subject factor. The results showed the main effect of time with F (4,116) = 2.839, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.09. There were no other significant main or interaction effects. The number of SCRs was reduced significantly during the induction, regardless of side or hypnotizability (Figure 2).

Click to view original image

Figure 2 Z-scored nonspecific responses during every two minutes of the induction phase averaged for the two sides of EDA measurement (on the left side) and the three hypnotizability groups (on the right side).

3.1.3 Electrodermal Activity (EDA) Patterns during Test Suggestions

We hypothesized differences in EDA patterns of the three hypnotizability groups during hypnotic suggestions. First, we used a two-way mixed ANOVA to test raw SCL measured from the right-side. The nine suggestions were used as the within-subject factor, and hypnotizability was used as the between-subject factor. The results displayed a significant main effect of suggestions with F (8,272) = 6.00, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15. The level of arousal changed significantly from one test suggestion to the other. A suggestion hypnotizability interaction effect was also found, F (16,272) = 3.14, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16 (Figure 3, left side).

We also analyzed differences in EDA patterns during hypnotic suggestions on the left side, using the three hypnotizability groups. Similar to the right-side results, the two-way mixed ANOVA, with the suggestions as the within-subject factor and hypnotizability as the between-subject factor, yielded a significant main effect of suggestions with F (8,256) = 4.53, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12. A suggestion hypnotizability interaction effect was also detected, F (16,256) = 3.14, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14 (Figure 3, right side).

These results demonstrated that EDA changes significantly during the different test suggestions and that this change is modulated by hypnotizability.

Click to view original image

Figure 3 SCL during the test suggestion phase, measured from the left and right sides.

3.1.4 Laterality during Test Suggestions

We hypothesized lateral differences during the suggestion phase of hypnosis, modulated by hypnotizability. To test this hypothesis, we applied two-way mixed ANOVA. We used average laterality during the test suggestions as the within-subject factor and hypnotizability as the between-subject factor. They yielded no significant effects (Figure 4). Electrodermal laterality does not seem to change significantly from suggestion to suggestion. Also, there is no significant difference among the hypnotizability groups. Although, it is clear from Figure 4 that high hypnotizables remained left-dominant throughout the hypnosis, while medium and low hypnotizables were right-side dominant.

Click to view original image

Figure 4 The laterality coefficient during the test suggestion phase. Positive numbers represent left side dominance, while negative numbers represent right side dominance.

3.2 Study 2

We performed a follow-up study to show the reproducibility of the most prominent effect found in study 1, namely, the gradual decrease in the level of skin conductance during hypnotic induction. We also examined the differences in this decrease in high, medium, and low hypnotizables. Two-way mixed ANOVA was calculated for each of the five sessions (Figure 5). We found no significant effects of hypnotizability in any of the sessions. The main effect of time was clear in the first session [F (3,10) = 7.32, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.42], the second session [F (3,13) = 5.90, p = 0.026, ηp2 = 0.31], the third session [F (5,70) = 6.08, p = 0.012, ηp2 = 0.30], and the fifth session [F (6. 72) = 5.48, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.31]. The fourth session on the other hand yielded no significant effect of time; although, this result could probably be due to the high variability of SCL. As seen in Figure 5, there was a gradual decrease in the average SCL during the induction phase, characteristic for all the hypnotizability groups, except for the low hypnotizables in session 4.

Click to view original image

Figure 5 Skin conductance level during the induction period of the five hypnosis sessions of the experiment. Solid black lines represent high hypnotizables, dashed grey lines represent medium hypnotizables, and solid grey lines represent low hypnotizables.

4. Discussion

4.1 EDA Levels during the Induction Phase

By measuring electrodermal activity in the induction and/or test suggestion phases of hypnosis, we identified typical electrodermal attributes related to the hypnotic state. The most prominent of these characteristics is the reduction in skin conductance level (SCL). Several studies have reported similar conclusions (Table 1).

During hypnosis induction, we observed a consistent decrease in skin conductance level across the 10-minute induction phase (Study 1). This effect was bilateral and characteristically different for the three hypnotizability groups. For the low and high hypnotizables, a variable electrodermal activity (EDA) pattern was detected; whereas, in medium hypnotizables, EDA gradually decreased throughout the induction phase. Reduction of arousal in hypnosis may be one of the important factors leading to therapeutic success in treating disorders associated with higher sympathetic arousal [7].

In our follow-up study (Study 2), we intended to reproduce the above findings. Five consecutive measurements from the same subjects demonstrated that the EDA reduction effect of the hypnotic induction remained pronounced for all hypnotizability groups (Figure 5).

4.2 EDA Laterality during Hypnotic Induction

Our results show evident bilateral differences during the hypnotic induction phase. High hypnotizables display left-side dominance, while low hypnotizables display right-side dominance. A number of previous studies had also highlighted these bilateral differences [19,20]. Our previous study also reported lateral differences during active-alert induction [4].

The medium hypnotizables showed a synchronous EDA activity of the two sides (Figure 1). Picard and colleagues (2015) [21] suggested a high correlation between the left and right sides with respect to EDA [21]. This high correlation has been confirmed in a number of studies (Kasos et al., under review) [13].

However, in high and low hypnotizables, EDA diverged on the two sides and stayed separated for the whole duration of the induction phase (Figure 1). The divergence of the two sides could be an indication of psychological distress. Picard observed right-side dominance in situations when the self was threatened [21]. Translating this to a hypnosis situation for low hypnotizables, they could be experiencing induction as a threatening situation, having to give up control to the hypnotist. This may be causing them to be distressed.

In contrast, high hypnotizables showed a strong left dominance (Figure 1). This may be explained by the multiple arousal theory [21]. According to this, positive emotions would cause EDA to be either close to synchronous or left-side dominant. For high hypnotizables, the induction process could be a positive experience. In addition, Gruzelier’s induction theory hypothesizes left-side hemispheric dominance at the beginning of induction [22]. Another study focuses on the verbal processing of induction, which in right-handed subjects would lead to left hemispheric dominance [23]. The amygdala is the foremost contributor to EDA and is mostly concerned with processing emotional information [24]. Hence, we hypothesize a strong emotional component behind the observed lateral differences during the induction process.

4.3 EDA Levels and Laterality during Test Suggestions

During test suggestions in Study 1, we observed that arousal levels fluctuated from one suggestion to the other, as reported previously [25]. The arousal level of high hypnotizables was higher during the hallucination suggestion and communication inhibition suggestion, confirming the findings from prior research [26]. Elevated levels of arousal may be explained by the pronounced cognitive effort required in responding to these suggestions. This implies that responding to suggestions requires considerable effort, as suggested by proponents of the dissociative experience theory and the social cognitive theory [27,28].

Contrary to the induction phase, lateral disposition during test suggestion was not significantly different among the three hypnotizability groups. Figure 3 shows that high hypnotizables remain left-side dominant, while medium and low hypnotizables remain right-side dominant for the whole duration of the suggestion phase. The suggestions, which are harder to respond to, such as hallucination, cause a more prominent left dominance in high hypnotizables.

The above results imply that responding to more difficult suggestions, such as hallucinations and communication inhibition, comes with a price that high hypnotizables showing higher arousal and a more left-sided electrodermal activation.

4.4 Non Specific SCRs

We hypothesized that a lower number of nonspecific skin conductance responses (SCRs) would be present at the end of the induction phase compared to the beginning. We also predicted that this effect would be modulated by hypnotizability. Our study confirmed that SCRs are fewer at the end of the induction. However, we found no evidence for differences based on hypnotizability. A reduced number of nonspecific responses could be explained by the nature of the hypnotic induction. During the induction, attention is mainly focused on the hypnotist and the inner experiences, with reduced peripheral awareness, resulting in fewer non-intended responses.

5. Limitations

The limitations of the present paper include the homogeneity of subjects in terms of their gender, age, and race. Our research could have benefitted from a higher number of participants.

6. Conclusion

In this article, we review the correlation between hypnosis and electrodermal activity (EDA) from the past 90 years of studies. We report the laterality and hypnotizability effects of electrodermal activity, during hypnotic induction and suggestion phases.

Most studies have highlighted lowered skin conductance level (SCL) during hypnosis, than pre- or post-hypnosis or in control conditions; however, contradictory findings have also been reported. In our study, we observed a prominent, bilateral reduction of SCL throughout the hypnotic induction phase, regardless of the level of hypnotizability. We also replicated this effect consistently in five independent hypnosis sessions.

Only a couple of studies have previously investigated bilateral EDA during hypnosis, with contradictory results. Our results highlight substantial differences in laterality during the hypnotic induction phase, with patterns characteristically differing depending on hypnotizability. Laterality differs throughout the hypnosis phase—high hypnotizables remained left-side dominant, whereas, medium and low hypnotizables were right-side dominant.

Nonspecific skin conductance responses (NS-SCRs) appear spontaneously, not related to any specific event. According to literature, the number of theseNS-SCRs is fewer during hypnosis than in control conditions. We, too, observe a decreasing number of SCRs in the induction phase. NS-SCR frequency typically shows great individual variety, with high levels of arousal, resulting in a higher frequency of NS-SCRs. Also, some findings indicate that high hypnotizables have less NS-SCRs than low hypnotizables. In contrast, we found no evidence for differences in the rate of NS-SCRs in relation to hypnotizability.

We conclude that arousal is reduced bilaterally during hypnotic induction and is persistent across different levels of hypnotizability. At the same time, lateral differences produce unique EDA patterns in the induction phase, defining high, medium, and low hypnotizables. The post-induction phase produces EDA that varies with suggestions. Typically, difficult suggestions produce higher arousal. Thus, our findings are novel, in terms of lateral differences of EDA in high versus low hypnotizables in the hypnotic induction phase. These results provide an objective, psychophysiological evidence for both, the multiple arousal theory and the left-side hemispheric dominance suggested by the induction theory of hypnosis.

On the basis of our findings, we strongly support that bilateral measurements should be used in hypnosis research. The ability to analyze laterality differences adds valuable information regarding the experiences of hypnosis participants. The changes that take place in a matter of a few minutes, altering one’s state of consciousness, make hypnosis induction a magnificent model situation to study electrodermal laterality.

Acknowledgments

This work was accomplished with support of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences (through project: LP-2018-21/2018). We highly appreciate the opportunity to use Obimon EDA prototype devices in our study and are most grateful for all technical support of Obimon Systems Ltd. We appreciate the support of the Faculty of Education and Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, and support of the Hungarian Ministry of Human Capacities through the student scholarships awarded to Luca Csirmaz and Szabolcs Zimonyi by the New National Excellence Program. We would like to thank Éva Bányai, András Költő, Balazs Nyíri (the hypnotists) for their support, and Alexandra Kasos for her invaluable comments on the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Krisztian Kasos: data collection, data analysis, statistical analysis, prepearing and writing manuscript. Luca Csirmaz: data collection, data analysis, writing and contributing to manuscript, Fanni Vikor: data collection, data analysis, contributing to manuscript, Szabolcs Zimonyi: data collection, Katalin Varga: providing essential theoretical knowledge regarding hypnosis, organizing and supervising hypnosis sessions, Anna Szekely: statistical analysis, writing and prepearing manuscript.

Funding

Hungarian Academy of Sciences (through project: LP-2018-21/2018).

Competing Interests

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

References

  1. Elkins GR, Barabasz AF, Council JR, Spiegel D. Advancing research and practice: The revised APA division 30 definition of hypnosis. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 2015; 63: 1-9. [CrossRef]
  2. Pekala RJ. Quantifying consciousness: An empirical approach; 1991. [CrossRef]
  3. Piccione C, Hilgard ER, Zimbardo PG. On the degree of stability of measured hypnotizability over a 25-year period. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989; 56: 289. [CrossRef]
  4. Kasos K, Kekecs Z, Kasos E, Szekely A, Varga K. Bilateral electrodermal activity in the active–alert hypnotic induction. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 2018; 66: 282-297. [CrossRef]
  5. Bowers KS. The Waterloo-Stanford Group C (WSGC) scale of hypnotic susceptibility: Normative and comparative data. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1993; 41: 35-46. [CrossRef]
  6. Bowers KS. Waterloo-stanford group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form C: Manual and response booklet. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1998; 46: 250-268. [CrossRef]
  7. Flammer E, Bongartz W. On the efficacy of hypnosis: A meta‐analytic study. Contemp Hypn. 2003; 20: 179-197. [CrossRef]
  8. Forbes EJ, Pekala RJ. Psychophysiological effects of several stress management techniques. Psychol Rep. 1993; 72: 19-27. [CrossRef]
  9. Yüksel R, Ozcan O, Dane S. The effects of hypnosis on heart rate variability. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 2013; 61: 162-171. [CrossRef]
  10. Moser G. The role of hypnotherapy for the treatment of inflammatory bowel diseases. Expert Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2014; 8: 601-606. [CrossRef]
  11. Kanji N, White A, Ernst E. Autogenic training to reduce anxiety in nursing students: Randomized controlled trial. J Adv Nurs. 2006; 53: 729-735. [CrossRef]
  12. Chen X, Yang R, Ge L, Luo J, Lv R. Hypnosis in the treatment of major depression: An analysis of heart rate variability. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 2017; 65: 52-63. [CrossRef]
  13. Kasos K, Zimonyi S, Gonye B, Koteles F. Obimon: An open-source device enabling group measurement of electrodermal activity. Psychophysiology. 2019; 56: e13374. [CrossRef]
  14. Boucsein W, Fowles DC, Grimnes S, Ben-Shakhar G, Roth WT, et al. Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology. 2012; 49: 1017-1034. [CrossRef]
  15. Dawson ME, Schell AM, Filion DL. The electrodermal system. Handbook of psychophysiology, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press; 2000. p. 200-223.
  16. Cacioppo J, Tassinary L, Berntson G. Handbook of psychophysiology; 2007.
  17. Shor RE, Orne EC. Norms on the Harvard group scale of hypnotic susceptibility, form A. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1963; 11: 39-47. [CrossRef]
  18. Benedek M, Kaernbach C. Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology. 2010; 47: 647-658. [CrossRef]
  19. Gruzelier JH, Brow TD. Psychophysiological evidence for a state theory of hypnosis and susceptibility. J Psychosom Res. 1985; 29: 287-302. [CrossRef]
  20. Gruzelier J, Allison J, Conway A. A psychophysiological differentiation between hypnotic behaviour and simulation. Int J Psychophysiol. 1988; 6: 331-338. [CrossRef]
  21. Picard RW, Fedor S, Ayzenberg Y. Multiple arousal theory and daily-life electrodermal activity asymmetry. Emot Rev. 2016; 8: 62-75. [CrossRef]
  22. Gruzelier J. A working model of the neurophysiology of hypnosis: A review of evidence. Contempor Hypn. 1998; 15: 3-21. [CrossRef]
  23. Jasiukaitis P, Nouriani B, Hugdahl K, Spiegel D. Relateralizing hypnosis: Or, have we been barking up the wrong hemisphere? Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1997; 45: 158-177. [CrossRef]
  24. Mangina CA, Beuzeron-Mangina JH. Direct electrical stimulation of specific human brain structures and bilateral electrodermal activity. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996; 22: 1-8. [CrossRef]
  25. Tart CT. Hypnotic depth and basal skin resistance. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1963; 11: 81-92. [CrossRef]
  26. Barber TX, Coules J. Electrical skin conductance and galvanic skin response during “hypnosis”. Int J Clin Exp Hyp. 1959; 7: 79-92. [CrossRef]
  27. Kihlstrom J. Alternatives. 1985; 385-418. [CrossRef]
  28. Spanos NP. Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological interpretation of amnesia, analgesia, and “trance logic”. Behav Brain Sci. 1986; 9: 449-467. [CrossRef]
Newsletter
Download PDF Download Full-Text XML Download Citation
0 0

TOP